Application of the Transtheoretical Model to Identify Physical Activity-Related Psychological Variables in Disabled Adults

Article information

Asian J Kinesiol. 2022;24(3):13-21
Publication date (electronic) : 2022 July 31
doi : https://doi.org/10.15758/ajk.2022.24.3.13
1Department of Sport Science, Seoul National University of Science and Technology, Republic of Korea
2Department of Physical Education, Seoul National University, Republic of Korea
*Correspondence: Youngho Kim, Department of Sport Science, Seoul National University of Science and Technology, Gongneung-ro 232, Nowon-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea; Tel: +82-02-970-6275; E-mail: yk01@seoultech.ac.kr
Received 2022 April 26; Revised 2022 May 25; Accepted 2022 July 31.

Abstract

OBJECTIVES

The current study aimed at identifying the theoretical relation of the transtheoretical model (TTM) to explain physical activity in Korean adults with physical disabilities.

METHODS

A total of 194 participants (Age Mean = 48.65 years) were voluntarily recruited for this study. Dissemination sources for participant recruitment included: (a) a press release issued through the authors’ university; (b) recruitment flyers posted on various websites (e.g., Independent Living Centers, Rehabilitation Research and Training Centers, Veteran’s Administration Hospitals); and (c) announcements made through and in conjunction with the Rehabilitation Research and Training Center on Health and Wellness consortium members. Through these procedures, 194 adults (80.83%) completed the survey form; there were 113 males (58.3%) and 81 females (41.7%). The remaining 46 (19.17%) were excluded because they did not complete the survey form or return.

RESULTS

Results indicated that the TTM constructs assessed were significantly (P < 0.001) associated with the stages of change for physical activity. The largest portion of variance was derived from the behavioral processes of change (ƞ2 = .40), followed by self-efficacy (ƞ2 = .30), the pros for exercise (ƞ2 = .19), the cognitive processes of change and the cons for exercise (ƞ2 = .16, respectively). Moreover, four discriminant functions (i.e., composite scores of the predictors) were produced in the first DDF analysis. These accounted for 71.0% (Wilks’ Λ = .31, χ2 [56] = 368.04, P < .001), 20.9% (Wilks’ Λ = .66, χ2 [39] = 129.91, P < 0.001), 6.3% (Wilks’ Λ = .88, χ2 [24] = 39.39, P < 0.05), and 1.8% (Wilks’ Λ = .97, χ2 [11] = 9.04, P = .62), respectively, of the between-group (stage of change) variability.

CONCLUSIONS

The results provide further cross-sectional support for the internal validity of the transtheoretical model, as the processes of change, self-efficacy, and decisional balance were important variables of a stage of change for physical activity, Additionally, the study is in general agreement with existing evidence among nondisabled populations and, therefore, it supports the external validation of TTM to a unique and understudied population segment.

Introduction

Many adults with physical disabilities needlessly experience hypokinetic diseases (i.e., diseases associated with disuse and inactivity), thus placing themselves at increased risk for sedentary death syndrome (SeDS). SeDS represents, “... sedentary lifestyle-mediated disorders that ultimately result in increased mortality” [1]. These disorders include angina, chronic pain, depression, heart disease, obesity, sleep apnea, stroke, and difficulty with the control of and recovery from various types of cancer, to name only a few. It is estimated that 70% of the U.S. population is affected by SeDS, and people with physical disabilities are among the most vulnerable [2,3].

Regular physical activity allows most people to decrease, delay, or avoid all together with their risk of experiencing hypokinetic diseases and SeDS [4]. Depending on the exercise definition used, 77– 88% of adults with physical disabilities do not exercise at a level sufficient to avoid hypokinetic diseases and SeDS [5].

Most research aimed at understanding why the majority of adults with physical disabilities do not exercise has been atheoretical, with a number of studies identifying or prioritizing study participants’ self-perceived barriers to exercise involvement [6,7]. Others have written about the benefits of exercise for those with disabilities and provided “how to exercise” information [8]. However, very little is known about theoretically based and practically important behavioral strategies that might be useful for encouraging and supporting exercise initiation and/or maintenance among adults with disabilities [9,10]. This priority area is featured in Healthy People 2010, which for the first time in the Healthy People series included an entire chapter on “Disability and Secondary Conditions” [11]. Moreover, within a separate chapter on “Physical Activity and Fitness,” the need for research targeting the elimination of disparities observed in physical activity is highlighted for various target groups, including people with disabilities.

Given there is little known about the factors influencing adults with physical disabilities’ physical activity [12,13], and in an effort to improve this situation, exercise scientists have been encouraged to examine the exercise behavior of adults with physical disabilities using contemporary psychosocial theories [9]. One such theory is the transtheoretical model (TTM) of behavior change [14]. TTM is comprised of four main dimensions: (a) stages of change, of which, depending on the behavior, five or six are hypothesized; (b) processes of change, of which five behavioral and five cognitive processes are hypothesized; (c) self-efficacy; and (d) decisional balance [15]. Conceptually, the stages of change seek to capture the temporal, motivational, and constancy dimensions of behavior change. The processes of change are the activities, events, and strategies that help people successfully change their behavior. A person’s situation-specific self-confidence in the face of barriers constitutes self-efficacy. And decisional balance suggests that a person will not adopt or maintain a regular exercise program unless her/his pros exceed her/his cons [16].

In a meta-analysis of 91 TTM studies in the exercise domain, no study was found that examined TTM’s constructs among adults with physical disabilities [17]. However, subsequent to the collection and review of studies used in the meta-analysis, one study has been published that examined the stage of change dimension (only) of TTM among adults with type-2 diabetes [18]. This study found that those in the more advanced stages of exercise behavior change, relative to those in the earlier stages, reported feeling less hopelessness and emotion-based coping responses on standardized questionnaires. Given these initially promising results, and due to its success among other population segments, TTM has been identified as a potentially useful model for understanding the physical activity of adults with physical disabilities [9]. However, due to the psychosocial dynamics experienced by adults with physical disabilities, before TTM is widely adopted or applied in intervention studies aimed at this population segment, its relevance and utility must first be examined within this population segment.

In addition to TTM and its associated constructs, a need for examining other variables and constructs relative to those proposed by TTM has been identified [19]. As noted, exercise decisional balance and self-efficacy are the most often studied construct in relation to people with disabilities [20,21]. In psychology, such psychological constructs are thought to be insurmountable obstacles that reside either within the individual or the environment and which interfere with the satisfaction of a need [22].

The purpose of this study was to examine the theorized associations of TTM of behavior change constructs (behavioral and cognitive processes of change, decisional balance, and self-efficacy) by stage of change for exercise behavior among individuals with physical disabilities. The specific research question of interest was: How are behavioral, cognitive, and environmental factors (i.e., processes of change, self-efficacy, and decisional balance) differentially associated with the stage of change for physical activity among adults with physical disabilities?

Methods

Participants

A total of 194 participants were voluntarily recruited for this study. Dissemination sources for participant recruitment included: (a) a press release issued through the authors’ university; (b) recruitment flyers posted on various websites (e.g., Independent Living Centers, Rehabilitation Research and Training Centers; and (c) announcements made through and in conjunction with the Rehabilitation Research and Training Center on Health and Wellness consortium members. Such recruitment occurred over 4 months. Participants who expressed an interest in the study during this time period were added to a wait-list database. Through these procedures, 240 adults with physical disabilities consented to participate in the study. Of those, 194 adults (80.83%) completed the survey form; there were 113 males (58.3%) and 81 females (41.7%). The remaining 46 (19.17%) were excluded because they did not complete the survey form or return. All participants provided their informed consent in compliance with the approval of the Institutional Review Board and the study was carried out in accordance with the International Declaration of Helsinki guidelines.

Measures

The stage of physical activity change scale, developed by Marcus, Selby, Niaura, and Rossi [23] was translated into Korean. In this questionnaire, the stage of physical activity was assessed using the 5-item dichotomous (yes/no) scale related to regular physical activity and intentions. Individuals have been categorized into one of the five stages of physical activity change described above. In a pilot phase of the test, test-retest reliability was carried out as a measure of instrument stability and achieved.85 on the Korean scale [24].

The decisional balance scale is a 10-items questionnaire that has been developed by Plotnikoff, Blanchard, Hotz, and Rhodes [25]. The scale was revised for the Korean version in the study, and the English version was used in the Malaysia-based sample. The scale consists of two sub-scales (pros and cons with five items each). The 5-point Likert scale was used, and participants were required to respond on the basis of their preference from “not at all important” to “extremely important”. Two main components of the decisional balance scale are the pros and cons, which represent the positive and negative aspects of the behavioral changes of the individual. The internal reliability of the Korean scale was reported as 0.89 for pros and 0.88 for cons [24].

Bandura [26] developed the self-efficacy scale for physical activity and was revised for the Korean and English versions of this study. The participants were asked to indicate on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (cannot do) to 3 (moderately certain can do) through intermediate levels of assurance, then to 5 (certain can do). Individuals rated in one space under a column labeled ‘‘confidence’’ how confident they were that they could perform exercise routines regularly (three or more times a week) under the different circumstances. Internal reliability was 0.91 on the Korean scale [24].

The processes of change scale developed by Nigg, Norman, Rossi, and Benisovich [27], is a 30-items question, that was revised into the Korean version. Similarly, the English version was used for the Malaysian sample. By using the 5-point Likert scale, students were asked to answer from “never (1)” to “repeatedly (5)”. In this study, cognitive processes and behavioral processes were influenced by second-order factors. The five components in cognitive processes are dramatic relief, consciousness-raising, environmental re-evaluation, self-re-evaluation, and social liberation, whereas the five components in behavioral are counter conditioning, helping the relationship, self-liberation, reinforcement management, and stimulus control. The internal reliabilities of the Korean version were 0.84 for cognitive processes of change and 0.87 for behavioral processes of change [24].

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics along with univariate F-tests and accompanying post hoc analyses were initially calculated for each TTM construct. Bivariate correlation coefficients for each major TTM construct were generated and examined for multicollinearity. Two direct discriminant function (DDF) analyses were conducted from which a classification matrix was generated (33). The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS version 26.0) was used for all data analysis.

Results

Descriptive statistics and univariate analyses

The majority of participants reported being in the maintenance stage of change (n = 78, 40.2%), followed by action (n = 50, 25.8%), preparation (n = 46, 23.7%), contemplation (n = 12, 6.21%), and precontemplation (n = 8, 4.10%). The means, standard deviations, F-tests, and Tukey post hoc contrasts for the behavioral and cognitive processes of change, self-efficacy, and decisional balance across the five stages of change are shown in <Table 1>.

Comparison of means and SDs of the TTM constructs across the stages of change.

All of the major constructs assessed were significantly (P < 0.001) associated with the stages of change for physical activity. The largest portion of variance was derived from the behavioral processes of change (ƞ2 = .40), followed by self-efficacy (ƞ2 = .30), the pros for exercise (ƞ2 = .19), the cognitive processes of change, and the cons for exercise (ƞ2 = .16, respectively). The correlation matrix for this set of variables is shown in <Table 2>. Notably, all correlation coefficients were < .70, which suggests multicollinearity was not a major issue.

Correlation among the TTM constructs.

After adjusting alpha using the Bonferroni correction (P < .05/10 = P < .05), all of the individual processes of change were significantly associated with the stages of change (P < .001) except dramatic relief (P > .05). In descending order of importance, the individual processes of change accounting for the most variance were counter conditioning (ƞ2 = .45), self-liberation (ƞ2 = .31), self-reevaluation (ƞ2 = .28), stimulus control (ƞ2 = .27), contingency management (ƞ2 = .23), consciousness raising (ƞ2 = .13), helping relationships (ƞ2 = 0.13), environmental reevaluation and social liberation (ƞ2 = .08, respectively), and dramatic relief (ƞ2 = .04). F-tests and Tukey post hoc contrasts for each of these variables are shown in <Table 2>.

The correlation coefficients among this set of variables (not shown) ranged from .22 to .72, with all but two of the 56 possible bivariate correlations being < .70. The two bivariate correlation coefficients > .70 were between contingency management and stimulus control (r = .71), and self-liberation and self-reevaluation (r = .72).

Direct discriminant function analysis

Four discriminant functions (i.e., composite scores of the predictors) were produced in the first DDF analysis. These accounted for 71.0% (Wilks’ Λ = .31, χ2 [56] = 368.04, P < .001), 20.9% (Wilks’ Λ = .66, χ2 [39] = 129.91, P < 0.001), 6.3% (Wilks’ Λ = .88, χ2 [24] = 39.39, P < 0.05), and 1.8% (Wilks’ Λ = .97, χ2 [11] = 9.04, P = .62), respectively, of the between-group (stage of change) variability. The structure matrix for each discriminant function, along with canonical correlations and eigenvalues are shown in <Table 3>.

Discriminant function analysis for the TTM constructs

After adjusting for group size differences, the stage of change classification probabilities revealed maintenance (90.2%), precontemplation (73.8%), and contemplation (55.2%) were the most reliably predicted stages. The least reliably predicted stages were preparation (23.8%) and action (15.8%). The overall classification accuracy across the stages of change was 70.8% <Table 4>.

Classification accuracy within and cross the stage of change

The DDF analysis was repeated. The results were generally supportive of the first DDF. Specifically, in this analysis the four discriminant functions generated accounted for 75.4% (Wilks’ Λ = .33, χ2 [52] = 341.84, P < .001), 18.2% (Wilks’ Λ = .71, χ2 [36] = 105.03, P < .001), 4.5% (Wilks’ Λ = .91, χ2 [22] = 29.25, P = .14), and 1.9% (Wilks’ Λ = .97, χ2 [10] = 8.83, P = .55), respectively, of the between-group (stage of change) variability. The eigenvalues and canonical correlations were 1.14 and .73 (first function), .28 and .46 (second function), .07 and .25 (third function), and .03 and .17 (fourth function), respectively. The order and magnitude of each structure coefficient remained the same (though, with the barriers excluded, the values varied slightly for the second through fourth functions). After adjusting for group size differences, the stage of change classification probabilities in this analysis revealed maintenance (91.3%), precontemplation (73.8%), and contemplation (48.3%) were the most reliably predicted stages. As before, the least reliably predicted stages were preparation (23.8%) and action (5.3%). The overall classification accuracy across the stages of change was 69.6%.

Discussion

The study results support the following initial observations. First, the behavioral processes of change (as both a higher order construct and as five individual constructs) along with self-efficacy improved in linear sequence from precontemplation (low) to maintenance (high) and, as hypothesized, these were the most important concomitants of the stages of change for physical activity. Counter conditioning, an individual behavioral change process strategy, was the single most important predictor of stage of change in both univariate and multivariate analyses. Second, the cognitive processes (as both a higher order construct and as five individual constructs) improved sequentially from precontemplation (low) to preparation (high), where they peaked, before descending in the action and maintenance stages (curvilinear pattern). The only exception to this was the pattern observed for social liberation, which followed a linear trend. Third, the cross-over point in decisional balance scores was observed in the preparation stage, which replicates a consistent finding observed in previous research (27). Fourth, those in contemplation perceived the most barriers to exercise, followed, in order, by those in action, preparation, pre- contemplation, and maintenance.

Using the 10 individual processes of change, self-efficacy, pros, cons, and exercise barriers to classify participants into their respective stage of change for exercise behavior, the most reliably predicted stages were maintenance (90.2%), precontemplation (73.8%), and contemplation (55.2%), with an overall classification accuracy of 70.8%. When this analysis was repeated without including the exercise barriers, the overall classification accuracy dropped by only 1.2%.

The bivariate correlation coefficients among predictor variables support the conceptual independence of the TTM constructs and exercise barriers. Of particular note is the 0.67 correlation observed between the behavioral and cognitive processes of change. Dating back to the initial TTM research in the area of smoking cessation where a 0.77 correlation was observed between these two constructs [28], and in the original TTM research in the exercise domain where a 0.91 correlation was observed, there has been some concern for the lack of empirical support for the conceptual distinction between these two constructs [29]. Within our sample, there was an empirically supported distinction between the higher-order behavioral and cognitive processes of change.

Relative to previous studies employing the full TTM among different population segments, and using similar multivariate statistical techniques, the stage of change for exercise behavior classification accuracy was higher in the present study (69.6% with exercise barriers excluded). In previous studies, the classification accuracy has been between 50% and 64.4% [29, 30]. The enhanced classification accuracy observed in the present study supports the relevance of TTM for this population segment. When the exercise barriers were included, there was a small increase (+1.2%) in the overall stage of change for physical activity classification accuracy (70.8%). Exercise barriers had the highest canonical correlation in the second discriminant function and therefore do appear to play an important and independent role across the hypothesized stages of change; particularly in univariate analysis and for those in the contemplation, preparation, and action stages of change. Because those in precontemplation were non-exercisers with no intention of starting an exercise program, it seems plausible that the exercise barriers queried lacked relevance for those in this stage of change.

This was a cross-sectional study, and therefore developmental trends in stage of change for exercise behavior cannot be observed. Longitudinal designs are recommended in order to examine the stability of different physical activity predictors across time. Second, actual physical activity behavior was not measured in this study. Future studies should attempt to measure both physical activity and stages of change to better establish the concurrent validity of the results. Third, the behavioral and psychological questionnaires used in the study rely on the self-report format. The results, therefore, may be subject to self-report bias, resulting in some unspecified amount of misclassification as to the participants’ actual levels of physical activity.

This is the first attempt to examine stage of change for physical activity among adults with physical disabilities on the basis of the full TTM. As the behavioral and cognitive processes of change, self-efficacy, and decisional balance were important variables of stage of change for physical activity, the results offer further cross-sectional support for the internal validity of TTM. Overall, the results are in general agreement with existing evidence among nondisabled populations and, therefore, they support the external validation of TTM to a unique and understudied population segment. This suggests behavioral strategies derived from TTM (e.g., gathering information, getting social support, making a commitment, making substitutions, taking advantage of social mores, using cues) may all positively influence adults with physical disabilities’ stage of change for physical activity. Moreover, and consistent with theory, these behavioral strategies appear to be differentially important across the hypothesized stages of change. It is hoped that this initial research will spawn the development of theory-based and empirically supported physical activity intervention strategies and programs directed toward adults with physical disabilities.

Notes

The authors declare no conflict of interests

Funding

This work was supported by the Ministry of Education of the Republic of Korea and the National Research Foundation of Korea(NRF-2019S1A5B5A07093627)

References

1. Lees SJ, Booth FW. Sedentary death syndrome, Canadian Journal of Applied Physiology. 2004;29(4):447–60.
2. Jeong JH, Yu JO. Prevalence and influencing factors of metabolic syndrome among persons with physical disabilities, Asian Nursing Research. 2018;12(1):50–55.
3. Rimmer JH. Physical activity for people with disabilities, The Lancet. 2012;380(9838):193–195.
4. Wang BW, Remay DR, Schettler JD, Hubert HB, Fries JF. Postponed development of disability in elderly runners: a 13-year longitudinal study, Archives of Internal Medicine. 2002;162:2285–2294.
5. U. S. Department of Health and Human Services. Physical activity and fitness. In: Healthy People 2010, 2nd Ed., With Understanding and Improving Health and Objectives for Improving Health (2 Vols.) Washington, DC: U. S. Government Printing Office; 2000. p. 3–39.
6. Kinne S, Patrick DL, Maher EJ. Correlates of exercise maintenance among people with mobility impairments, Disability Rehabilitation. 1999;21:15–22.
7. Rimmer JH, Rubin SS, Braddock D. Barriers to exercise in African American women with physical disabilities, Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2000;81:182–188.
8. Markides KS, Rudkin L, Goodwin GS. Disability in older adults 2: physical activity as prevention, Behavioral Medicine. 1999;24:157–168.
9. Kosma M, Cardinal BJ, Rintala P. Motivating individuals with disabilities to be physically active, Quest. 2002;54:116–132.
10. Taylor WC, Barowski P, Young D. Physical activity interventions in low-income, ethnic minority, and populations with disability, American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 1998;15:334–343.
11. U. S. Department of Health and Human Services. Physical activity and fitness. In: Healthy People 2020, 4th Ed., With Understanding and Improving Health and Objectives for Improving Health (2 Vols.) Washington, DC: U. S. Government Printing Office; 2020. p. 3–39.
12. Dustine JL, Painter P, Franklin BA, Morgan D, Pitetti BH, Roberts SO. Physical activity for the chronically ill and disabled, Sports Medicine. 2000;30:207–219.
13. Rimmer JH, Brabbock D. Health promotion for people with physical, cognitive, and sensory disabilities: an emerging national priority, American Journal of Health Promotion. 2002;16:220–224.
14. Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC. Stage and processes of self-change of smoking: Toward and integrative model. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1983;51:390–395.
15. Han H, Gabriel PK, Kohl HW. Application of the transtheoretical model to sedentary behaviors and its association with physical activity status. PLoS ONE 2017;12e0176330.
16. Kang SJ, Kim YH. Application of the transtheoretical model to identify predictors of physical activity transition in university students. Journal of Sport Psychology 2017;26:6–11.
17. Marshall SJ, Biddle SJ. The transtheoretical model of behavior change: a meta-analysis of applications to physical activity and exercise. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 2001;23(4):229–2462.
18. Kirk A, MacMillan F, Webster N. Application of the transtheoretical model to physical activity in older adults with Type 2 diabetes and/or cardiovascular disease. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 2010;11:320–324.
19. Elezim A, Elezi G, Gontarev S, Georgiev G. Application of the transtheoretical model (TTM) to exercise behavior among Macedonian college students. Journal of Human Sport and Exercise 2019;15:1–12.
20. McGlumphy KC, Gill AK, Hasson RE. Application of the transtheoretical model to physical activity and exercise behaviors in African-American adolescents. American Journal of Health Behavior 2019;43:119–132.
21. Nigg C, Harmon B, Jiang Y, Ginis KAM, Motl RW, Dishman R. Temporal sequencing of physical activity change constructs within the transtheoretical model. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 45:1–7.
22. Cardinal BJ, Tuominen KJ, Rintala P. Cross-cultural comparison of American and Finnish college students’ exercise behavior using transtheoretical model constructs. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 2004;75:92–101.
23. Marcus BH, Selby VC, Niaura RS, Rossi JS. Self-efficacy and the stage of exercise behavior change. Research Quarterly foe Exercise and Sport, 1992;63:60–66.
24. Kim YH. Application of the transtheoretical model to identify psychological constructs influencing exercise behavior: a questionnaire survey. International Journal of Nursing Studies 2007;44:934–944.
25. Plotnikoff RC, Blanchard C, Hotz SB, Rhodes R. Validation of the decisional balance scales in the exercise domain from the transtheoretical model: a longitudinal test. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise Science 2001;5:191–206.
26. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: the exercise and control 1997. W.H. Freeman. New York:
27. Nigg CR, Norman GJ, Rossi JS, Benisovich SV. Processes of exercise behavior change: Redeveloping the scale. In : Poster presented at SBM; 1999. San Diego, CA.
28. Romain AJ, Horwath C, Bernard P. Prediction of physical activity level using processes of change from the transtheoretical model: experiential, behavioral, or an interaction effect? American Journal of Health Promotion 2018;32:16–23.
29. Kim YH, Cardinal BJ. Effects of a transtheoretical modelbased stage-matched intervention to promote physical activity among Korean adults, International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology. 2009;9:259–273.
30. Kim YH, Cardinal BJ, Lee JY. Understanding exercise behavior among Korean adults: A test of the transtheoretical model. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine 2006;13:295–303.

Article information Continued

Table 1.

Comparison of means and SDs of the TTM constructs across the stages of change.

Variable Stage of change
F ƞ2 Post hoc
PC CO PR AC MA
1 M 9.0 11.7 12.5 13.2 14.6 34.2** .30 PC<CO,PR<MA;
SD 4.2 4.2 1.8 2.0 2.4 PC<AC
2 M 7.6 9.7 10.6 9.9 10.1 19.1** .19 PC<CO,PR,AC,MA
SD 2.7 2.4 1.8 1.9 1.8
3 M 6.0 5.4 4.7 5.3 4.4 11.2** .12 PC>PR,MA;
SD 2.2 1.6 1.7 1.8 1.4 MA<CO,AC
4 M 41.4 53.9 56.9 53.4 52.2 15.0** .16 PC<CO,PR,AC,MA
SD 11.2 10.7 10.1 12.3 10.5
5 M 8.4 9.6 10.9 9.8 9.0 12.1** .13 PC<CO,PR,AC,MA
SD 3.0 2.8 2.2 3.0 3.1
6 M 8.4 9.6 10.9 9.8 9.9 2.9* .04 PC<PR
SD 3.8 3.4 3.1 3.5 3.2
7 M 7.4 10.3 10.3 9.4 9.0 6.6** .08 PC<CO,PR,AC,MA
SD 3.1 3.2 3.6 3.4 3.0
8 M 9.0 12.9 13.6 12.6 13.2 30.4** .28 PC<CO,PR,AC,MA
SD 3.3 2.2 2.1 3.2 2.4
9 M 8.4 9.7 10.0 10.1 10.2 6.5* .08 PC<AC,MA
SD 2.3 2.4 2.9 2.4 2.7
10 M 36.1 44.7 49.6 55.5 57.7 52.9** .40 PC<CO<AC,MA;
SD 10.3 10.3 10.9 9.9 9.7 PC<PR,MA
11 M 7.5 9.4 10.7 11.3 11.3 23.9** .23 PC<CO,PR,AC,MA;
SD 2.7 2.6 2.6 2.5 2.9 CO<AC,MA
12 M 7.3 8.5 9.8 11.7 12.8 61.8** .45 PC<PR<AC,MA;
SD 2.7 2.3 2.5 2.6 2.5 CO<AC,MA
13 M 6.5 7.4 8.8 9.1 9.4 11.6** .13 PC<PR,AC,MA;
SD 2.4 3.0 2.8 3.3 3.3 CO<MA
14 M 8.9 11.5 12.1 13.8 13.3 35.0** .31 PC<CO,PR,AC,MA;
SD 3.0 2.4 2.6 2.3 2.6 CO<AC,MA
15 M 6.0 8.0 8.4 9.8 10.0 29.2** .27 PC<CO,PR,CO,MA;
SD 2.2 2.7 2.7 2.4 2.8 CO<AC,MA

1=self-efficacy, 2=pros, 3=cons, 4=cognitive processes, 5=conscious raising, 6=dramatic relief, 7=environmental reevaluation, 8=self-reevaluation, 9=social liberation, 10=behavioral processes, 11=contingency management, 12=counter conditioning, 13=helping relationships, 14=self liberation, 15=stimulus control

PC=precontemplation, CO=contemplation, PR=preparation, AC=action, MA=maintenance

*

P < .05,

**

P <.001

Table 2.

Correlation among the TTM constructs.

Variables Self-efficacy Pros Cons Cognitive processes Behavioral processes
Self-efficacy
Pros .42
Cons -.35 -.24
Cognitive processes .34 .52 -.17
Behavioral processes .61 .48 -.29 .67
*

All correlations ≥ 0.17 are significant (P < 0.01).

Table 3.

Discriminant function analysis for the TTM constructs

Predictor variables Structure coefficients
Function 1 Function 2 Function 3 Function 4
Counter conditioning .82* -.09 .01 -.01
Self-efficacy .61* .14 -.22 -.19
Self-liberation .59* .30 .32 -.07
Stimulus control .56* .14 .07 -.18
Contingency management .49* .24 .10 .23
Helping relationships .35* .09 .0 .27
Social liberation .25* .19 -.04 -.03
Self-reevaluation .48 .56* -.29 -.01
Consciousness raising .25 .49* .03 .20
Environmental reevaluation .12 .45* -.04 -.03
Pros .39 .42* -.17 .26
Cons -.32 -.07 .43* -.18
Dramatic relief .03 .29 .09 .47*
Canonical R .73 .50 .31 .17
Eigenvalue 1.15 .34 .10 .03
*

Signifies largest absolute correlation between each variable and any discriminant function

Table 4.

Classification accuracy within and cross the stage of change

Observed stage of change Predicted stage of change
PC CO PR AC MA Total
PC 73.8(45) 6.6(4) 1.6(1) 1.6(1) 16.4(10) 18.9(61)
CO 6.9(2) 55.2(16) 10.3(3) 10.3(3) 17.2(5) 9.0(29)
PR 4.8(1) 28.6(6) 23.8(5) 4.8(1) 38.1(8) 6.5(21)
AC 7.9(3) 5.3(2) 0.0(0) 15.8(6) 71.1(27) 11.8(38)
MA 5.2(9) 2.3(4) 0.0(0) 2.3(4) 98.2(156) 53,7(173)
Total 18.6(60) 9.8(32) 3.0(9) 4.7(15) 64.0(206) 70.8(322)

Cell values are reported as percentages with frequencies reported in parentheses.

Percentages may not total 100 due to rounding.

PC=precontemplation; CO=contemplation; PR=preparation; AC=action; MA=maintenance.